banner
News center
Our ISO certification demonstrates our commitment to excellence.

The effects of age, size, and cage complexity on the behaviour of farmed female chinchillas (Chinchilla lanigera)

Jun 06, 2023

Scientific Reports volume 13, Article number: 6108 (2023) Cite this article

474 Accesses

1 Citations

Metrics details

Even though chinchillas have been farmed for a century, there are not many studies concerning their behaviour in captivity or their optimal housing conditions, both of which are important factors in the assessment of their welfare. This study aimed to evaluate the effect of different cage types on chinchillas’ behaviour and their reactions towards humans. Female chinchillas (n = 12) were kept in three types of cages: standard with a wire floor (S); standard with a deep litter floor of shavings (SR); and enlarged with a deep litter floor of shavings (LR). Animals spent 11 weeks in each type of cage. The chinchillas’ reactions toward humans were observed via intruder test. Ethograms were prepared based on round-the-clock video recordings. The activity of the chinchillas was compared, taking into account the different cage types and the animals’ varying responses to the hand test. The generalized ordered logistic regression model was used to ascertain whether cage type has an effect on a chinchilla's behaviour towards humans. To compare the time distribution of various activity between chinchillas, the non-parametric Scheirer–Ray–Hare test was used. Animals kept in LR cages presented significantly less timid reactions in comparison to those kept in S and SR cages. The chinchillas spent most of their time resting (68% of the day), in locomotion (23%), and eating or drinking (8%); they spent only 1% on grooming behaviour. Cage enrichment generally reduced the fear of humans. However, the average chinchilla response to the hand test was classified in each type of cage as "cautious". Analyses of the ethograms indicated that the chinchillas were active mostly during the dark stage of the day. In conclusion, the larger cage size and its enrichment (particularly litter) reduced the fearfulness and passivity of the animals, which could be evidence of better welfare conditions.

Under the Five Domains Model1, current animal welfare science takes into account nutrition, environment, health, behavioural interactions, and mental state. One aspect of the physical environment is the housing system, which can have a positive or negative effect on animals’ mental state1. In addition to the importance of animal housing conditions and animal health, these recommendations also emphasize the expression of normal behavioural patterns. They indicate that it is important to take abnormal behaviours (e.g. excessive aggression or stereotypes) into consideration in animal welfare assessments2. These principles underpin the EU-founded Welfare Quality® project, which is the basis of farm animal welfare scoring. On this basis, the European Fur Breeders Association prepared on-farm welfare-assessment protocols for farmed fur animals (foxes and minks)3,4,5. Such detailed protocols have not yet been developed for the chinchilla6.

The natural habitat of the long-tailed chinchilla (Chinchilla lanigera, Bennne, 1829) covers barren, arid and rugged areas of transverse mountain chains in north-central Chile. These animals are nocturnal and live in herds; however, there is relatively little information about their natural biology (i.e. social behaviour, predators, etc.). As a fur animal, this species has been threatened for many years by hunting and poaching. The wild chinchilla population is continuing to decline due to human activities, such as the pet trade, grazing by cattle and goats, mining, firewood extraction, and El Niño events7. Currently, the wild population is estimated at 5350 mature individuals7, the distribution of which is restricted to only a few small and fragmented colonies8. Therefore, the wild long-tailed chinchilla is endangered under criteria B2ab(i,ii,iii) and was added to the IUCN Red List of Threatened Species in 20157. On the other hand, due to its very valuable fur, the long-tailed chinchilla (Chinchilla lanigera Bennett, 1829) has been domesticated since the 1920s and kept as pets and laboratory animals9. Although they have been farmed for a century, there are not many studies concerning their behaviour or comprehensively detailing their living conditions10,11,12,13, both of which are important factors in the assessment of their welfare and degree of domestication. Different guidelines for keeping and caring for chinchillas can be found in the regulations of the European Commission14, national regulations of particular countries, and suggestions from animal rights organizations or breeders' associations Furthermore, there are important disagreements between the different guidelines. For example, the Polish National Chinchilla Breeders Association recommends cages with netting or a solid floor with minimum dimensions (width, length, and height) of 0.40 m × 0.45 m × 0.34 m (0.06 m3)15, the Canadian Standard Guidelines for the Operation of Chinchilla Ranches16 require 2200 cubic inches (approx. 0.04 m3) for each animal, while the German Veterinary Association for Animal Welfare17 advises the use of group housing systems with a minimum volume of 3 m3 for two animals (min. 0.5 wide and 1.5 m high) and at least 0.5 m3 for every additional animal.

In addition to the cage dimensions, the type of flooring also greatly influences the natural behaviours animals exhibit, such as scratching and dust bathing, when in contact with bedding material18,19,20. It was found that adding various substrates and structural enrichment to cages (shelves, wooden blocks/sticks, deep litter, etc.) enriches this environment, which reduces animals’ undesirable behaviour, such as chinchillas’ fur-chewing12 and fear and aggressive reactions in farmed foxes21. Environmental enrichment can improve both the physiological and psychological welfare of captive animals, which can be assessed by noting the increased expression of natural behaviour and decreased expression of abnormal behaviours. However, a key factor contributing to how a captive animal interacts with its environment is its relationship with humans22. A study in mice kept in standard cages has shown impaired brain development, abnormal repetitive behaviours (stereotypies), and an anxious behavioural profile, all of which can be lessened by making the cage environment more stimulating23. Additionally, cage enrichment (e.g. gnawing objects for foxes) allows farm animals to relieve stress more easily, which may improve the animals’ responses to humans21.

Farm animals' negative reactions toward humans, which can be caused by limited contact with humans (e.g. only at weaning or vaccinations)24, have serious economic, practical, and welfare implications25,26.

The most common method of testing fur animals’ reactions to humans is the "stick test"27. Based on an animal's immediate reaction to a wooden spatula being inserted into the cage through the mesh, individuals can be categorized as fearful, curious (sometimes referred to as "confident") or aggressive. This test is also recommended in mink and fox welfare assessment protocols4,5. However, "Trapezov's hand test" is recommended as a more sensitive alternative. In this behavioural test, the cage lid is opened, a gloved hand is slipped in, and the mink is touched if possible27,28. On this basis, it can be hypothesized that the size and complexity of chinchilla cages influence their behaviour and responses to humans. Therefore, the study objective was to verify this hypothesis and evaluate the effect of housing conditions on the human-animal relationship. This could help to improve chinchillas’ welfare and provide valuable information on the behaviour this species.

The reported experiment was approved by the First Local Ethical Committee on Animal Testing at Jagiellonian University in Kraków (Licence no. 45/2014), and all methods and animal treatment used in the study were performed according to the guidelines and regulations stated in Directive 2010/63/EU on the protection of animals used for scientific purposes and the Act 2015/266 of the Republic of Poland on the protection of animals used for scientific or educational purposes. The study reported in the manuscript follows the recommendations in the ARRIVE guidelines.

The domestic chinchillas (standard colour, sexually mature, virgin, unrelated females; eight months old; initial body weight 593 ± 53.3 g; n = 12) were obtained one month before starting the study from a commercial breeding farm ("Raba" Chinchilla Breeding Farm, Myślenice, Poland). They were taken to the chinchilla breeding facility of the University of Agriculture in Kraków, where they were quarantined for four weeks in the same housing system as used at commercial chinchilla farms. Chinchilla females were housed individually in stainless-steel cages (0.40 m width × 0.50 m length × 0.35 m height) with a wire floor. Throughout the study, the animals were exposed to a controlled temperature in the range of 18–22 °C and a photoperiod of 14.5 h light (total natural and artificial)/9.5 h dark.

The animals were randomly divided into three groups (four animals per group) and assigned to three experimental conditions (cage type); all cages were equipped with a sand bath and a wooden block:

Standard cage with a wire floor (standard, S; 0.40 m width × 0.50 m length × 0.34 m height, Fig. 1a), equipped with a ceramic plate under the feeder to reduce the loss of fodder;

Standard cage with a deep litter floor of shavings and a grid platform on the rear wall (enriched standard, SR; 0.40 m width × 0.50 m length × 0.34 m height; Fig. 1b);

Enlarged cage with a deep litter floor of shavings and two grid platforms on the rear and side walls (larger and enriched, LR; length 0.60 m × width 0.50 m × height 0.68 m; Fig. 1c).

The chinchilla housing types used in the research: (a) standard cage (S) with a wire floor, equipped with a ceramic plate under the feeder to reduce the loss of fodder. (b) enriched standard cage (SR) with a deep litter floor of shavings and a grid platform on the rear wall. (c) enlarged and enriched cage (LR) with a deep litter floor of shavings and two grid platforms on the rear and side walls.

Each type of cage was placed in one row.

The experiment was carried out between June and March in three blocks of 11 weeks each (A: June–September; B: October–December; C: January–March). Each block consisted of one week of acclimatization and ten weeks of observing the animal in a given cage type). After each block, the same animal was transferred to the next cage type, according to a design of the experiment (Table 1), which allowed for 12 repetitions for each block, so a total of 36 repetitions. The environmental conditions were constant throughout the whole study. In order to reduce individual differences, all animals in the study were already accustomed to handling, and they were treated the same as in standard commercial settings.

A modified version of Trapezov's hand test27,28,29 (with modifications for this study), hereafter "the hand test", was used to categorize the responses of chinchillas to the intrusion of the researcher's hand into their cages. To this end, before starting the experiment the researcher (the same person who tested the animals during the experiment) performed preliminary tests on 40 other animals to identify behavioural patterns and develop a rating scale. To prevent the habituation of the animals, the researcher did not work with these chinchillas regularly but only visited the breeding facility occasionally during testing. When performing the test, the researcher put one hand on the open front of the cage and moved it slowly inside. The reaction of the chinchillas to handling attempts was recorded according to a five-point scale (Table 2). Test duration was about 15–30 s, depending on how much the animal interacted with the researcher. The test was carried out in each cage for each animal once a week for the 8 months of the study with a total 30 repetitions per animal. The mean scores of this test were used to classify the animals’ responses: 1.00–1.80 = confident; 1.81–2.60 = cautious; 2.61–3.40 = timid; 3.41–4.20 = nervous; 4.21–5.00 = aggressive.

The maintenance behaviours were classified and analysed in terms of duration (behavioural states) according to Franchi et al.13, with a modification for this study (Table 3). The continuous behavioural observations of the chinchillas were simultaneously carried out using three infrared surveillance charge-coupled device (CDD) cameras (model AT TI560E; one camera was focused on the animals in four cages of one type) and recorded with a time-lapse digital recorder for the last five days of each animal's residence in a particular cage type. After the observation period, the video recordings (1080 h of recording) were analysed by the same person using General_RECPlayer v1.8 software. The duration of each behaviour was individually documented by the same observer. All observed behaviours were noted in the research protocol with an accuracy of ± 1 min. The obtained data made it possible to create an ethogram of female chinchillas kept in captivity.

The generalized ordered logistic regression (GOLR) model was used to assess the effect of different cage types on the chinchilla's behaviour30,31. The model was selected because of the ordered response of the chinchillas to the hand test experiment. The reference cage was S and the reference block was A. The scheme made it possible to assess the change of the chinchilla's response due to the move from the ‘worst’ to the ‘best’ cage and from the first to the second and third block. Because measurements were repeated with the same chinchillas, random effects were included in the model, taking into account the specific random effect that each animal has,

\({\pi }_{ij}\) is the probability of being in category \(j\) assuming that the explanatory variable is \(i\). In this case, the reference category is numbered \(M\). In the GOLR model, the logarithm of the odds ratio to be in category \(j\) compared to the reference category \(M\) equals

for \(j\in \left\{1, ... , M-1\right\}\), and \(i\) is the number of the covariate. As we observed four different reactions (scores) to the hand test, the level \(M=4\) was taken as the reference category. The possible covariates \({X}_{i}\) are (cage LR, block B), (cage LR, block C), (cage SR, block B), (cage SR, block C). The slopes \({\beta }_{j}\) depend on the response variable in the sense that they can be different for various levels \(j\) of the response variable even if the covariates are identical.

Taking into account the different types of cages and the animals’ varying responses to the hand test (confident, cautious, timid, nervous, or aggressive), the activity of the chinchillas was compared using the Scheirer–Ray–Hare test (SRH)—the non-parametric equivalence of the 2-way ANOVA32.

The authors confirm that: (1) the reported experiments were approved by the First Local Ethical Committee on Animal Testing at Jagiellonian University in Kraków (Licence no. 45/2014); (2) all methods and animal treatment used in the study were performed according to the guidelines and regulations stated in Directive 2010/63/EU on the protection of animals used for scientific purposes (http://data.europa.eu/eli/dir/2010/63/oj), and the Act 2015/266 of the Republic of Poland on the protection of animals used for scientific or educational purposes (O.J. 2015 pos. 266).

The study reported in the manuscript follows the recommendations in the ARRIVE guidelines (PLoS Biol 18(7): e3000411. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000411).

The estimates of the coefficients in the GOLR model [Eq. (1)] were displayed in Table 4. They show that there was a significant association between log odds and cage LR which implies there was a significant change in points from 4 to 1 when chinchillas were moved from cage S to LR, from 4 to 2 when chinchillas were moved from cage S to cages LR and SR, and from 4 to 3 when chinchillas were moved from cage S to LR. As regards blocks, the change from block A to B resulted in a significant change in points from 4 to 1. The change from 4 to 2 or 3 was only weakly associated with the change from block A to B or C. To sum up, chinchillas moved from cage S to LR exhibited a behavioural change towards humans from nervous to confident/cautious/timid. Being moved from block A to B resulted in the change from nervous to confident.

Averaged chinchilla responses to the hand test were classified in each type of cage as cautious (S: 2.4 ± 1.08; SR: 2.3 ± 0.99; LR: 1.9 ± 0.81). It should be noted that no individual in LR was classified as nervous. However, some individuals reacted strongly to cage change, e.g. U26 become more confident after transfers into SR and LR cages while U42 and U60 became nervous/timid after being moved from LR into S cages.

The chinchillas spent most of their time resting (68% of the day), followed by locomotion (23%), eating, and drinking (8%); they spent 1% of their time on grooming behaviour (Fig. 1). However, the type of cage affected the duration of the chinchillas’ maintenance behaviours. In comparison to cage S, animals housed in SR and LR cages spent more time on "extensive locomotion" but less on resting. Moreover, chinchillas in LR cages spent only ~ 10% as much time eating hay as those in other cage types (Table 5). A comparison of the chinchilla's activity based on hand test results showed that cautious chinchillas spent more time moving around (locomotion) than animals from other classes (Table 6). However, SRH tests showed that there was no difference between the distribution of time the animals spent in each form of activity (P = 0.92) for various cage types.

Analyses of the ethograms indicated that the chinchillas were active mostly during the dark stage of the day (between 9.30 p.m. and 7 a.m.). Significant regularity of eating behaviour was observed, with the highest hay consumption occurring immediately after the addition of a fresh portion of forage. Grooming activities were only observed between 7:00 a.m. and 9:30 p.m., with the highest intensity at about 4 p.m. (Fig. 2).

Patterns of the daily behaviour of caged chinchilla females. Cage category designations: S—standard with a wire floor; SR—enriched standard with a deep litter floor and a grid platform; LR—enlarged with a deep litter floor and two grid platforms. Behaviour category designations: R—Resting; L1—Moderate locomotion; L2—High locomotion; EP—Eating pellets; EH—Eating hay; EW—Drinking; GR—Self-directed activities.

One of the most important aspects of farm animals’ welfare is the proper human-animal relationship. Intruder tests/temperament/human intruder tests are used as a non-invasive method to quantify and evaluate animal personalities, and categorize them as fearful or shy and have been widely used for domestic, farm, and even wild animals.

Our preliminary research on chinchillas demonstrated that very few animals showed a pronounced response to the stick test, which made it unsuitable for assessing individual responses. The hand test gave much more unambiguous results and in our opinion it is more useful in research on chinchilla behaviour33.

Lapinski et al.'s comparison34 of the hand test and the empathic test in assessing the animal (chinchilla)-human relationship indicates that although results of both tests were similar (R = 0.4979), the reaction of the chinchillas in the first one were much more recognizable and clear.

Lack of fear of humans can reduce stress, and confident individuals endure farm conditions better35. The results of our experiment show that housing conditions influence chinchillas’ reactions to human intrusion, but the strength of these reactions depends on the individual animal. However, this interpretation requires us to take into account the limited number of studied individuals; the same ones were used for all three cage types. Therefore, the order of cage type could have affected the results. On the other hand, these observations are consistent with those carried out on many other species of farm, laboratory, and zoo animals, e.g. minks36, polar foxes37, pigs38, rats, and mice39, as well as non-human primates40. For example, enriching the cage environment by adding toys (gnawing objects) caused a significant changing of minks’ personalities, as estimated by empathic tests of the movement from "fearful" towards "confident", while such a reaction was not observed in "aggressive" ones12. Sha et al.40, based on observations of captive non-human primates (cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) and Goeldi's monkeys (Callimico goeldii)), suggest that environmental complexity and/or feeding enrichment) seem to have a larger effect on overall activity levels compared with the effect of larger enclosure sizes on increased species-typical behaviours. Similarly, goats in the enrichment group (afforded enrichments such as food presentation, the use of physical barriers, and the use of elevated areas) had a more excited reaction than the control group, which could be related to their cognitive state thanks to the effect of the enrichment41.

The domestication of animals (including chinchillas) involves the selection (intended for animal breeding) of individuals with a mild, balanced character (temperament) that can withstand farm conditions and tolerate human presence28,42. These animals generally have better reproductive and health parameters42,43,44. Nevertheless, domesticated animals still show avoidance towards human beings35,36,45,46. In the present study, the ‘appropriate behaviour’ of chinchillas, (understood as one of the principles of animal welfare, together with good feeding, good housing, and good health47,48) was confirmed by video analysis.

In the course of analysing the video recordings, an additional observation (not included in the research plan) was made that most animals were not only excited and waited at the cage door for the caretaker (not a researcher) at feeding time, but they also started to eat without fear of direct human presence.

In our opinion, this observation might be compared to the results of the feeding test, which is used, for example, to assess responses to human handling in the welfare protocol for foxes4,21. Our study also indicates that better responses to human handling are presented by individuals kept in cages with a deep litter floor. In litter cages, the amount of time spent on intensive activity (running) increased at the expense of rest (passivity), which may indicate that litter creates better conditions for movement. Moreover, it was found that chinchillas prefer a quiet, inner corner of the cage for a resting place, and in cages with a mesh floor they use solid areas (ceramic plates) as a resting place.

In the recordings of cages that contained litter, the chinchillas were observed to roll in sawdust. This behaviour (a dust bath) is a natural behaviour observed in chinchillas for fur maintenance. Moreover, Łapiński et al.12 noticed that fur-chewing cases decreased in solid-floor cages with litter. These observations support the guidelines that at least 25% of the accommodation floor should be solid for chinchillas14. Studies on rabbits confirm the beneficial influence of litter on the welfare of cage-housed animals49,50. Straw, which is used as bedding, plays a dual role: it is used as substrate enrichment and as an absorbent for droppings. However, rabbits choose litter so long as it is fresh, but they do not tolerate material spoiled by urine and faeces. Therefore, if regular litter replacement is not possible, then rabbits prefer the cleanliness and the dryness of a wire floor49,50. However, the litter material in chinchilla cages is wood shavings, and the production of urine and faeces by this species is lower than that of rabbits; therefore, our study supports the use of bedding in chinchilla caging to improve captive conditions. Our results also indicate that the type of cage (size and floor type) affects the chinchillas’ behaviour. Nevertheless, the cages used as a standard (S and SR) in our experiment have been widely used for decades in chinchilla breeding; they comply with the internal rules of many EU Member States51 and seem to have been accepted by most animals in the experiment. It should be noted that the CoE recommendation14 proposes that a cage's minimum cubic capacity for one individual is 0.50 m3, which is similar to foxes (0.56 m3) and is more than four times bigger than for minks (0.11 m3), even though minks and foxes are much larger than chinchillas. The fact that the minimum requirements are met by standard cages is also supported by the fact that in our experiment no stereotypical behaviours were observed in this caging system. However, these behaviours had been observed mostly during the night period in earlier studies by Franchi et al.13.

Moreover, chinchillas in LR cages spent less time eating hay than those in other types of cages. One potential explanation for this is that eating hay, apart from its nutritional function, is a form of play (activity), which can compensate for less space and stimuli in S and SR cages. It is known that eating hay, apart from its nutritional function, is a form of play (activity). Animals like to search for food (they choose the tastier parts of hay) even if it is readily available. Hay, straw, or grass satisfy guinea pigs’ and rabbits’ need to chew roughage39, which is also observed in chinchillas. In contrast, eating can be a response to boredom in humans52. Also, a study on minks indicated that animals without environmental enrichment consume more food than those with environmental enrichment53. Chronic inescapable boredom can be can be extremely detrimental to their welfare, and insufficient stimulation can harm neural, cognitive, and behavioural flexibility. Animals in captivity are at particular risk of spatial and temporal monotony, which can have important implications for their welfare54.

Analysis of the 24-h video recordings and ethograms generally confirms that chinchillas are crepuscular and nocturnal animals55, but Franchi et al.13 observed that sleeping and resting are the most frequently observed behaviours for these animals in captivity. In our study, sleeping and resting were mainly manifested in the light period and lasted 52.2 min/h, whilst locomotion lasted 3.7 min/h. In the dark period, locomotion lasted 27.7 min/h, and sleeping and resting lasted 25.2 min/h. However, we must emphasize that changes in the activity pattern occurred rapidly after the light was turned on or off.

In conclusion, the size of the cage and its contents (particularly litter) improves hand test results and seems to stimulate activity, which could be evidence of better welfare of chinchillas.

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author upon request.

Mellor, D. J. et al. The 2020 five domains model: including human–animal interactions in assessments of animal welfare. Animals 10, 1870. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10101870 (2020).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Rushen, J., Chapinal, N. & de Passillé, A. M. Automated monitoring of behavioral-based animal welfare indicators. Anim. Welf. 21, 339–350. https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.21.3.339 (2012).

Article CAS Google Scholar

Mononen, J. et al. The development of on-farm welfare assessment protocols for foxes and mink: the WelFur project. Anim. Welf. 21, 363–371. https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.21.3.363 (2012).

Article CAS Google Scholar

WelFur. Welfare assessment protocol for foxes. WelFur Consortium, Brussels, Belgium. https://www.sustainablefur.com/wp-content/uploads/2018/11/WelFur_fox_protocol_web_edition.pdf (2014).

WelFur. Welfare assessment protocol for minks. WelFur Consortium, Brussels, Belgium. https://www.sustainablefur.com/wp-content/uploads/2018/11/Mink_protocol_final_web_edition_light.pdf (2015).

Łapiński, S. Czy certyfikacja ferm zapewni hodowcom dobrostan [Will farm certification ensure the welfare of breeders?]. Inf. Hod. Szynszyli 34(2), 17–25 (2020) (in Polish).

Google Scholar

Roach, N. & Kennerley, R. Chinchilla lanigera. The IUCN red list of threatened species 2016: e.T4652A117975205. https://doi.org/10.2305/IUCN.UK.2016-2.RLTS.T4652A22190974.en (2016).

Faúndez, P. V., Oyarzún, Á. S. & Ramos, C. Z. Natural history of the Chinchilla genus (Bennett 1829). Considerations of their ecology, taxonomy and conservation status/Historia natural del género Chinchilla (Bennett 1829). Consideraciones de su ecología, taxonomía y estado de conservación. Gayana 78, 135–143. https://www.scielo.cl/pdf/gayana/v78n2/art08.pdf (2014).

Google Scholar

Spotorno, E., Zuleta, C. A., Valladares, J. P., Deane, A. L. & Jimènez, J. E. Chinchilla laniger. Mamm. Species 758, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1644/758 (2004).

Article Google Scholar

Ponzio, M. F., Busso, J. M., Ruiz, R. D. & Fiol de Cuneo, M. A survey assessment of the incidence of fur chewing in commercial chinchilla (Chinchilla lanigera) farms. Anim. Welf. 16, 471–479. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0962728600027408 (2007).

Article CAS Google Scholar

Ponzio, M. F. et al. Adrenal activity and anxiety-like behavior in fur-chewing chinchillas (Chinchilla lanigera). Horm Behav. 61, 758–762. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.03.017 (2012).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Łapiński, S., Lis, M. W., Wójcik, A., Migdał, Ł & Guja, I. Analysis of factors increasing the probability of fur chewing in chinchilla (Chinchilla laniegera) raised under farm conditions. Ann. Anim. Sci. 14, 189–195. https://doi.org/10.2478/aoas-2013-0067 (2014).

Article Google Scholar

Franchi, V., Alejandro, A. O. & Tadich, T. A. Fur chewing and other abnormal repetitive behaviors in chinchillas (Chinchilla lanigera), under commercial fur-farming conditions. J. Vet. Behav. 11, 60–64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2015.10.002 (2016).

Article Google Scholar

CoE. Standing Committee of the European Convention for the Protection of Animals Kept for Farming Purposes (T-AP). Recommendation Concerning Fur Animals, Adopted by the Standing Committee on 22 June 1999. https://www.coe.int/t/e/legal_affairs/legal_co-operation/biological_safety_and_use_of_animals/farming/T-AP%20(99)%2019%20E%20Hnadling%20of%20documents.pdf (1999).

KZHS. Kodeks dobrych praktyk w chowie i hodowli szynszyli w warunkach fermowych. [Code of the best practices in the breeding and housing of chinchillas in farm conditions]. National Chinchilla Breeders Association, Myślenice (2014) (in Polish).

OMAFRA. Standard Guidelines for the Operation of Chinchilla Ranches. Ministry of Agriculture, Food and Rural Affairs. Ontario. Canada. http://www.omafra.gov.on.ca/english/livestock/alternat/facts/chinguid.htm (2014) (Downloaded on 17 February 2020).

TVT. Merkblatt Nr. 153 - Heimtiere: Chinchillas (Stand: Sept. 2012). Tierärztliche Vereinigung für Tierschutz e.V. https://www.tierschutz-tvt.de/alle-merkblaetter-und-stellungnahmen/ (2012) (in German) (Downloaded on 17 February 2020).

Cruden, J., Cooper, J., Burman, O. & Whelan, G. Housing preferences of laboratory rabbits. in Report of the 2015 RSPCA/UFAW rodent and rabbit welfare Group meeting. Animal Technology and Welfare 15 (eds Hawkins, P., Atkinson, J., Birt, R., Cruden, J., Duran, A., Herrmann, K., Leach, M., Lopez-Salesansky, N., McBride, A., Paiba, G., Roughan, J. & Huw, G.) 9–22. https://www.rspca.org.uk/webContent/staticImages/Downloads/RWM2015.pdf (Downloaded on 17th August 2020).

Fleming, P. A. et al. The contribution of qualitative behavioral assessment to appraisal of livestock welfare. Anim. Prod. Sci. 56, 1569–1578. https://doi.org/10.1071/AN15101 (2016).

Article Google Scholar

Szendro, Z. S. et al. A review of recent research outcomes on the housing of farmed domestic rabbits: Reproducing does. World Rabbit Sci. 27, 1–14. https://doi.org/10.4995/wrs.2019.10599 (2019).

Article Google Scholar

Łapiński, S., Pałka, S., Wrońska-Fortuna, D. & Guja, I. Effect of cage enrichment on the welfare of farmed foxes. Med. Weter 75, 665–668. https://doi.org/10.21521/mw.6242 (2019).

Article Google Scholar

Claxton, A. M. The potential of the human–animal relationship as an environmental enrichment for the welfare of zoo-housed animals. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 133, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2011.03.002 (2011).

Article Google Scholar

Wolfer, D. P. et al. Cage enrichment and mouse behaviour. Nature 432, 821–822. https://doi.org/10.1038/432821a (2004).

Article ADS CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Meagher, R. K., Duncan, I., Bechard, A. & Mason, G. J. Who's afraid of the big bad glove? Testing for fear and its correlates in mink. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 133, 254–264. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2011.05.009 (2011).

Article Google Scholar

Hemsworth, P. H. Human–animal interactions in livestock production. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 81, 185–198. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(02)00280-0 (2003).

Article Google Scholar

Jones, B. & Manteca, X. Practical strategies for improving farm animal welfare: An information resource. http://www.welfarequality.net/media/1003/information_resource.pdf (2009) (Downloaded on 9.03.2020).

Malmkvist, J. & Hansen, S. W. Generalization of fear in farm mink, Mustela vison, genetically selected for behavior towards humans. Anim. Behav. 64, 487–501. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2002.3058 (2002).

Article Google Scholar

Trapezov, O. V. Selected transformations of defensive reactions to man in American mink (Mustela vison Schreb). Genetika 23, 1120–1127 (1987) (in Russian).

CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Trapezov, O. V., Trapezova, L. I. & Sergeev, E. G. Effect of coat color mutations on behavioral polymorphism in farm populations of American minks (Mustela vison Schreber, 1777) and sables (Martes zibellina Linnaeus, 1758). Russ J Genet 44, 444–450. https://doi.org/10.1134/S1022795408040108 (2008).

Article CAS Google Scholar

McCullagh, P. & Nelder, J. A. Generalized Linear Models Monographs on Statistics and Applied Probability, 37 2nd edn, 511 (Chapman and Hall, 1989).

MATH Google Scholar

Williams, R. Generalized ordered logit/partial proportional odds models for ordinal dependent variables. Stata J. 6, 58–82. https://doi.org/10.1177/1536867X0600600104 (2006).

Article Google Scholar

Scheirer, J., Ray, W. S. & Hare, N. The analysis of ranked data derived from completely randomized factorial designs. Biometrics 32, 429–434. https://doi.org/10.2307/2529511 (1976).

Article CAS PubMed MATH Google Scholar

Makowski, A. Wpływ temperamentu na parametry rozrodu szynszyli (Chinchilla lanigera) [The influence of the temperament on reproductive parameters in chinchilla (Chinchilla lanigera)]. Master's thesis, University of Agriculture in Kraków. University of Agriculture in Krakow, Theses Repository. https://apd.urk.edu.pl/diplomas/14499/ (2016).

Łapiński, S., Niedbała, P. & Matusevičius, P. Comparison of two behavioral tests on the chinchillaʼs response to humans. in International Conference Microbiota and Animal: Interaction, Health, Welfare and Production. Kaunas, 29 September, 2022, pp. 45–46. https://hdl.handle.net/20.500.12512/115737 (2022).

Browning, H. The natural behavior debate: Two conceptions of animal welfare. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 23, 325–337. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2019.1672552 (2020).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Henriksen, B. I., Møller, S. H. & Malmkvist, J. Test of temperament in mink is influenced by a shelf in the front of the cage and the stimulus size rather than by features of the test person. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 233, 105155. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2020.105155 (2020).

Article Google Scholar

Gacek, L. Test empatyczny—Nowy test behawioralny dla lisów polarnych. [Empathic test—New behavioral test for polar foxes]. Prz. Hod. 5, 4–6. http://ph.ptz.icm.edu.pl/wp-content/uploads/2017/12/4-6-11.pdf (2002) (in Polish).

Google Scholar

Marcet-Rius, M. et al. The provision of toys to pigs can improve the human–animal relationship. Porcine Health Manag. 6, 29. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40813-020-00167-x (2020).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Baumans, V. Environmental enrichment for laboratory rodents and rabbits: Requirements of rodents, rabbits, and research. ILAR J. 46, 162–170. https://doi.org/10.1093/ilar.46.2.162 (2005).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Sha, J. C. M., Ismail, R., Marlena, D. & Lee, J. L. Environmental complexity and feeding enrichment can mitigate effects of space constraints in captive callitrichids. Lab Anim. 50, 137–144. https://doi.org/10.1177/0023677215589258 (2016).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Miranda-de la Lama, G. C. et al. Environmental enrichment and social rank affects the fear and stress response to regular handling of dairy goats. J. Vet. Behav. 8, 342–348. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2013.03.001 (2013).

Article Google Scholar

Kowalska, D., Bielański, P. & Pietras, M. The relationship between behavior with some productive traits of rabbits. J. Life Sci. 6, 657–662. https://doi.org/10.17265/1934-7391/2012.06.011 (2012).

Article Google Scholar

Acharya, R. Y., Hemsworth, P. H., Coleman, G. J. & Kinder, J. E. The animal-human interface in farm animal production: Animal fear, stress, reproduction and welfare. Animals (Basel) 12, 487. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani12040487 (2022).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Nikula, S., Smeds, K., Hietanen, H., Kenttämies, H., Ojala, M. Confident behavior and production traits - results from a field study of foxes. in Proceedings of Seventh International Scientific Congress in Fur Animal Production, Scientifur. 24, III-B, 99–102. http://ifasanet.org/PDF/vol24_4/I.pdf (2000).

Price, E. O. Behavioral aspects of animal domestication. Q. Rev. Biol. 59, 1–32. https://doi.org/10.1086/413673 (1984).

Article Google Scholar

Rushen, J., Taylor, A. A. & de Passilé, A. M. Domestic animals’ fear of humans and its effect on their welfare. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 65, 285–303. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(99)00089-1 (1999).

Article Google Scholar

Keeling, L., Evans, A., Forkman, B. & Kjaernes, U. Welfare quality® principles and criteria. In Improving Farm Animal Welfare (eds Blokhuis, H. et al.) 91–114 (Wageningen Academic Publishers, Wageningen, 2013).

Chapter Google Scholar

Botreau, R., Veissier, I., Butterworth, A., Bracke, M. B. M. & Keeling, L. J. Definition of criteria for overall assessment of animal welfare. Anim. Welf. 16, 225–228. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0962728600031390 (2007).

Article CAS Google Scholar

Bosco, A. D., Castellini, C. & Mugnai, C. Rearing rabbits on a wire net floor or straw litter: Behavior, growth and meat qualitative traits. Livest Prod. Sci. 75, 149–156. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0301-6226(01)00307-4 (2002).

Article Google Scholar

Morisse, J. P., Boilletot, E. & Martrenchar, A. Preference testing in intensively kept meat production rabbits for straw on wire grid floor. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 64, 71–80. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(99)00023-4 (1999).

Article Google Scholar

Dz, U. Obwieszczenie Ministra Rolnictwa i Rozwoju Wsi z dnia 19 września 2019 r. w sprawie ogłoszenia jednolitego tekstu rozporządzenia Ministra Rolnictwa i Rozwoju Wsi w sprawie minimalnych warunków utrzymywania gatunków zwierząt gospodarskich innych niż te, dla których normy ochrony zostały określone w przepisach Unii Europejskiej. [Announcement of the Minister of Agriculture and Rural Development of 19 September 2019 on the announcement of the consolidated text of the Ordinance of the Minister of Agriculture and Rural Development on minimum conditions for the maintenance of farm animal species other than those for which the standards of protection are laid down in the European Union regulations]. Dziennik Ustaw 2019, poz. 1966. https://isap.sejm.gov.pl/isap.nsf/download.xsp/WDU20190001966/O/D20191966.pdf (2019) (in Polish)

Moynihan, A. B. et al. Eaten up by boredom: Consuming food to escape awareness of the bored self. Front. Psychol. 6, 369. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00369 (2015).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Meagher, R. K. & Mason, G. J. Environmental enrichment reduces signs of boredom in caged mink. PLoS ONE 7, e49180. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0049180 (2012).

Article ADS CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Burn, C. C. Bestial boredom: A biological perspective on animal boredom and suggestions for its scientific investigation. Anim. Behav. 130, 141–151. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.06.006 (2017).

Article Google Scholar

Jiménez, J. E. The extirpation and current status of wild chinchillas Chinchilla lanigera and Chinchilla brevicaudata. Biol. Conserv. 77, 1–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/0006-3207(95)00116-6 (1996).

Article Google Scholar

Download references

Michael Timberlake and Katarzyna and Glen Cullen for professional linguistic proofreading of the manuscript; Wiktor Tabak for drawings of the cages pictures.

This research was supported by the Ministry of Education and Science of the Republic of Poland (Subject No. 020014-D015, University of Agriculture in Kraków, the University of Agriculture in Krakow).

Department of Zoology and Animal Welfare, University of Agriculture in Krakow, Al. Mickiewicza 21, 31-120, Kraków, Poland

Stanisław Łapiński, Katarzyna Markowska & Marcin W. Lis

Department of Animal Reproduction Anatomy and Genomics, University of Agriculture in Krakow, Al. Mickiewicza 21, 31-120, Kraków, Poland

Piotr Niedbała

Department of Applied Mathematics, University of Agriculture in Krakow, Al. Mickiewicza 21, 31-120, Kraków, Poland

Agnieszka Rutkowska

You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

S.Ł. conceived and designed the project. S.Ł., P.N., and K.M. performed the experiments. S.Ł., A.R., KM, and M.L. analysed the data. S.Ł., A.R., and M.L. wrote the manuscript. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Correspondence to Marcin W. Lis.

The authors declare no competing interests.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and Permissions

Łapiński, S., Niedbała, P., Markowska, K. et al. The effects of age, size, and cage complexity on the behaviour of farmed female chinchillas (Chinchilla lanigera). Sci Rep 13, 6108 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-32516-5

Download citation

Received: 04 August 2022

Accepted: 28 March 2023

Published: 14 April 2023

DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-32516-5

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.